Babies Don’t Need to Be Built: Alex Bollen on the Danger of the “Good Mother” Myth

Cultural prescriptions on play are strong. A good example of this is Margot Sunderland’s book What Every Parent Needs to Know. Sunderland, who is a child psychologist and psychotherapist, claims that parents need to “activate” the “PLAY system” and “SEEKING s…
Miss Joshua Miller · about 1 month ago · 4 minutes read


```html

The Myth of the Good Mother: Unpacking the Pressure to "Build Baby's Brain"

The Science of Play: More Myth Than Fact?

Cultural pressures surrounding play are immense. Child psychologist Margot Sunderland, in her book What Every Parent Needs to Know, urges parents to activate their child's "PLAY" and "SEEKING" brain systems, warning of a life less extraordinary if they fail. However, concrete evidence supporting these claims remains elusive. The authors of Understanding Child Development acknowledge a lack of convincing data linking play to significant cognitive gains, though social benefits appear more promising.

They ultimately conclude that the true value of play lies in its inherent enjoyment. Yet, even this simple pleasure is often warped into a "strenuous work project," as Alison Gopnik observes, particularly in American culture where middle-class parents feel justified in play only when it serves the "work" of parenting.

Prescriptive Play: A Modern Phenomenon

Sunderland's advice is remarkably prescriptive, advising against independent or child-led play. She even suggests that adult-child play is superior due to the adult's "more advanced frontal lobe functions." Parents are instructed to comment on, but not question, a child's play, lest they disrupt their "flow," and warned against choosing toys or leading play, which supposedly activates stress hormones.

This perspective clashes starkly with historical and anthropological evidence. Anthropologist David Lancy notes that parent-toddler play is "virtually non-existent" in most societies. The !Kung people, often seen as examples of "natural parenting," even believe playing with children could be detrimental.

Lancy finds the idea of "teaching" children to play "absurd" and "ridiculous." Historically, play was a communal activity linked to festivals, not childhood. Historian Gary Cross notes how industrialization shifted play from a communal activity to a childhood one, separating work and leisure.

The Evolution of Play: From Discouraged to Mandatory

The association of play with children didn't immediately translate into parental responsibility. In fact, playing with babies was once actively discouraged. Early 20th-century parenting manuals warned against playing with infants under six months, citing potential harm to their nervous systems and digestion.

The post-World War II era saw a dramatic shift, with play becoming a maternal duty. Psychologist Martha Wolfenstein dubbed this new obligation the "fun morality." Historian Peter Stearns attributes this to consumerism and advertising, while smaller families and limited outdoor play options also contributed.

The Burden of "Fun Morality"

While some mothers find play with their children rewarding, for others, it's a source of guilt, anxiety, and boredom. Expert advice often ignores this reality, intensifying the pressure. For example, Sunderland claims parental enjoyment of play is so inevitable that they'll crave more.

This expectation contrasts sharply with Victorian-era advice which cautioned against overstimulating children, warning of potential physical and emotional harm.

Brain Building: A Metaphor Gone Too Far?

Modern parenting is steeped in the metaphor of "building baby's brain." Programs like Vroom, funded by the Bezos Family Foundation, offer "science-based tips" for transforming everyday moments into "Brain Building Moments®."

This "more is better" approach is pervasive, with organizations like the NSPCC implying that every interaction missed is a missed opportunity for brain development. This is a logical fallacy; growth has limits. This constant pressure can leave parents, especially mothers, feeling perpetually inadequate.

Concepts like "brain architecture" and "serve and return," while presented as scientific fact, are far from settled science. Neuroscience is in its infancy, and the brain is not a house built from the bottom up, but a complex, dynamic organ constantly changing.

As Matthew Cobb, a neuroscientist, points out: "Brains, unlike any machine, have not been designed. They are organs that have evolved for over five hundred million years." Comparing the brain to a house vastly oversimplifies its complexity.

The power of the "brain-building" metaphor lies not in science, but in its alignment with Good Mother myths. It offers seemingly simple solutions to complex questions, but ultimately perpetuates unrealistic expectations and pressures on parents.

```